Escape Behavior Differences
in Cryptic and Aposematic Frogs
·
During one of our class presentation, we learned
all about aposematic coloration in frogs. Along with coloration the various
studies looked at relative toxicity, mating, and the relative predator preference
by color. One experiment conducted by Hegna, Saporito and Donnelly used
adorable clay models to mimic the exact shades of brown, red, and green seen in
three frog species as a way to test relative predation based on number of
attack. The clay models were placed on the forest floor and after a specified
time the experimenters simply counted the number of etch marks in the clay and
tallied each one as a predator attack. The experiment found that green was
attacked the most, red an intermediate number of times, and brown the least (Hegna,
Saporito and Donnelly).
·
It appears from this study that less predator
attempts were made on red frogs, likely due to their warning coloration acting
as a predator defense. However, Ozel and Stynoski have qualms regarding this
study’s over simplified approach since they failed to account for a crucial
component of prey-predator interaction: prey escape behavior.
·
This study investigates what the clay models
could not, differences in prey escape behavior when faced with an incoming
predator.
·
In most of the animal kingdom the Fight or
Flight instinctual response is actually Fight, Flight or Hide. This third component
highlights the importance of regulating resources whether that means not
wasting energy fleeing or not leaving a suitable/plentiful habitat. Escape
behaviors should only be performed when the threat of danger is immediate and
the calculation of when the risk is substantial enough to flee is determined by
several factors. Optimal escape theory of Ydenberg and Dill looks at the
trade-offs between staying in place and deciding to flee using a measurement known
as flight initiation distance (FID), or the distance from predator to prey when
the prey begins escape behavior (Ydenberg and Dill).
·
Aposematic coloration is a defense strategy
designed to warm potential predators of dangerous qualities of the prey, frequently
that they are poisonous, by displaying noticeable often bright colors. Cryptic coloration
takes an opposite approach in an effort to camouflage the prey and have it go
undetected. Based only on the dramatic differences in defense strategy it is
likely that escape behavior varies according to coloration.
·
The current study investigates differences in
escape behavior between the aposematic poison frog, Oophaga pumilio and the cryptic frog, Craugator bransfordii, both of which can be found in the forests of
Costa Rica (Ozel and Stynoski).
·
The experiment measured the distance between the
frog and the human observer at the moment that frog began escape flight. They
also recorded the distance traveled by the frog from initial flight spot to
final landing position, with turns and multiple hops accounted for.
·
Although it has often been suggested that there
are differences in escape behaviors between aposematic and cryptic frogs, this
study was the first to show in a field study that there is indeed reduced
escape behavior in aposematic frogs when compared to cryptic frogs.
·
This study not only compared cryptic to
aposematic but also analyzed the influence of sex and vocalization ability on escape
behavior by comparing the flight initiation distance of cryptic frogs, female
aposematic, non-vocalizing male aposematic, and vocalizing male aposematic
frogs. As we know from discussions in class vocalization in frogs is costly, it
takes up a lot of energy and is used not only in courtship but also in territory
defense. The results of this study showed that vocalizing aposematic males had
the lowest flight initiation distance of any group, and often failed to flee at
all. This behavior is likely a decision related to their anti-predator
mechanism combined with their territorial tendencies to stay put. Fleeing might
lose them their territory and reduce chance of mating which would lower
fitness. This explanation of the observed behavior is supported by the high
flight initiation distance seen in non-vocalizing aposematic males and
aposematic females. Aposematic males that did not flee frequently turned their
dorsum up towards the predator since this is the area where toxins are most
concentrated they might harm the predator without losing their position within
their territory.
·
This research adds to our knowledge of cryptic
versus aposematic coloration in frogs and their variation in strategy for
survival. The differences observed between sexes where males are more likely to defend a territory and females are more likely to flee since are often sought out and do not have to worry about courtship or losing mating opportunities as much as males do. The differences between vocalizing and non-vocalizing groups further illustrate the energy expense of vocalization (since they don't want to waste energy fleeing) as well at the importance of vocalization in establishing territory. The vocalizing males are willing to go to great lengths in order to defend their territory, whereas non-vocalizing males show behaviors more similar to the females.
By: Ozel, L.D. and Stynoski, J.L.
·
Hegna, R.H., Saporito, R.A., and Donnelly, M.A.
(2013). Not all colors are equal: predation and color polytypism in the
aposematic poison frog Oophaga pulilio. Evol
Ecology. 27: 831-845.
·
Ozel, L.D. and Stynoski, J.L. (2011).
Differences in Escape Behavior between Cryptic and an Aposematic Litter Frog. Journal of Herpetology. 45(3): 395-398.
·
Ydenberg, R.C. and Dill, L.M. 1986. The
economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior.
16:229-249.
4 comments:
I wonder what kind of reaction predators would have on a clay model of a Bombinidae frog in the Unken reflex position.
That's a good question Chris!My guess is that they would be greatly deterred.
Great follow-up to the presentation on aposematic frogs. How cool that the males adjust their predator response when they're calling!
Hey Shann!!! I am just studying for our final tomorrow and on the topic of aposomatic coloration I wanted to let you know that a species called atelopus, part of the family Bufonidae (in the order Anura) have aposomatic coloration and this is also seen in a different family within Anura called the poison dart frogs or dendrobates. This is a great example of convergent evolution within the same order of Anura. This blog post of yours is very interesting, I think the frogs must have high levels of testerone or maybe they are just very cocky and don't get scared by predators because they know they are poison and think they are invincible!!! See you tomorrow Shan!
Post a Comment